Some past research has questioned or even evaluated the
effectiveness of using fear appeals in anti-tobacco campaigns. Unfortunately, one major element that is
included in these appeals is often overlooked…the element of disgust. This raises the question: Is the use of
disgusting images in fear appeals a more effective way to change smoking
attitudes, intentions, and behaviors globally?
What exactly is disgust?
In order to discuss disgust, I will provide a quick overview of
disgust. Disgust is described as an unpleasant
emotion that often evokes specific facial expressions and feelings of nausea (Davey, Forster, & Mayhew, 1993). Nabi (2002) simply describes disgust as being
“grossed out”. Typically disgust makes
individuals want to withdraw or avoid the item that is bringing forth the
emotion (Davey, Forster, & Mayhew, 1993). Disgust may
have developed as a protective mechanism to prevent humans from ingesting toxic
or poisonous items (see Haidt, McCauley, & Rozin, 1994). In other words, it may aid in basic human
survival.
There are three different types of disgust: core disgust (Rozin, Haidt, & McCauley, 1993), animal reminder
disgust (Rozin & Fallon, 1987) and moral
disgust (Nabi, 1998). Core disgust is connected to food and
ingestion. Animal reminder disgust is
associated with anything that reminds us of our mortality or that we are
animals. This includes disgust evoking
stimuli related to death or bodily organs and fluids. Moral disgust is linked to acts and behaviors
that are perceived to be culturally or socially unacceptable. For example, some sexual acts are viewed as
disgusting because they are deemed to be socially unacceptable.
In addition, Haidt, McCauley, and Rozin (1994) found seven
domains of disgust elicitors. These
include: food, animals, body products, sex, envelope violations, death, and
hygiene (Haidt, McCauley, & Rozin, 1994)
Now, I return to my original question. Is the use of disgusting images in fear
appeals a more effective way to change smoking attitudes, intentions, and
behaviors globally? Much like with studies
on fear appeals, the findings are mixed.
Some studies argue that disgust combined with fear is overwhelming and
leads to defensive reactions (Leshner, Bolls, & Wise, 2011). Others argue that these types of appeals are
effective across cultures (Durkin, Bayly, Cotter, Mullin, & Wakefield, 2013).
Recent policies and regulations for tobacco control have
mandated the inclusion of graphic health warnings on all cigarette packaging
(see the Family Smoking Prevention Control Act, 2009). Many countries have implemented these
policies.
Below, I provide a few examples of these graphic health
warnings. Caution, the images below are
pretty graphic!
Images Source: World
Health Organization (2014). WHO FCTC
health warnings database. Accessed Dec
3, 2014.
Venezuela Ad: Smoking causes bad breath, tooth loss, and
oral cancer.
Iran Ad: Smoking will cause irremediable damages to the eye
tissue, vision central nerves and finally will result in blindness.
Malaysia Ad: Cigarette causes neck cancer.
Soo...what is your reaction to these images? Do you think they are effective?
Interestingly, tobacco companies in the United States filed
litigation against the use of these ads in 2011 (Strasser, Tang, Romer, Jepson, & Cappella, 2012). So for now
the use of these warning labels have been temporarily blocked in the United
States.
I think the new graphic health warnings that employ disgust
and fear are extremely interesting. The
issue introduces several global health questions about the effectiveness of
emotional appeals and the role of policy and large corporations in health.
Works Cited:
Davey, G. L.,
Forster, L., & Mayhew, G. (1993). Familial resemblance in disgust
sensitivity and animal phobias. Behavior Research and Therapy , 31,
41-50.
Durkin, S., Bayly, M., Cotter, T., Mullin, S., &
Wakefield, M. (2013). Potential effectiveness of anti-smoking advertisement
types in ten low and middle income countries: Do demographics, smoking
characteristics and cultural difference matter? Social Science &
Medicine , 98, 204-213.
Haidt, J., McCauley, C., & Rozin, P. (1994). Individual
differences in sensitivity to disgust: A scale sampling seven domains of
disgust elicitors. Personality and Individual Differences , 16
(5), 701 -713.
Leshner, G., Bolls, P., & Wise, K. (2011). Motivated
processing of fear appeal and disgust images in televised anti-tobacco ads. Journal
of Media Psychology , 23 (2), 77-89.
Nabi, R. (2002). Anger, fear, uncertainty, and attitudes: A
test of the cognitive-functional model. Communication Monographs ,
69 (3), 204-216.
Nabi, R. L. (1998). The effect of disgust-eliciting visuals
on attitudes toward animal experimentation. Communication Quarterly ,
46 (4), 472-484.
Rozin, P., & Fallon, A. (1987). A perspective on
disgust. Psychological Review , 94, 23-41.
Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (1993). Disgust.
In M. Lewis, & J. Haviland (Eds.), The handbook of emotions (pp.
575-594). New York: Guilford.
Strasser, A. A., Tang, K. Z., Romer, D., Jepson, C., &
Cappella, J. N. (2012). Graphic warning labels in cigarette advertisements:
Recall and viewing patterns. American Journal of Preventative Medicine ,
43 (1), 41-47.
Wow, Tasha, I have never thought about disgust like this before! There sure does need to be a lot of research on the effectiveness of these advertisements. Good thing you are doing the job. I think that if I saw these images constantly on my cigarette packages, I would be more motivated to quit smoking. But that is me personally.
ReplyDeleteHere is another news article on the use of packaging to get the message that smoking is bad across:
http://www.bbc.com/news/magazine-30061952
Here, it also discusses the infringement on intellectual property rights. Philip Morris, a cigarette manufacturer, says in this article, “we oppose excessively large health warnings that infringe on our intellectual property rights and have been proven not to reduce smoking rates.” This particular company also spent 5.25 million euros lobbying in Europe in response to these sort of health warnings. To me, this is yet another example of how transnational corporations ignore human decency to survive in the neo-liberal economy.
It is hard for me to have an opinion on whether or not these advertisements are effective. When I see the picture, instead of looking at it closely and letting it sink in on how tobacco can cause the grotesque images, I am distracted by how gross the picture is. Instead, I try to ignore the picture as much as possible, which in turn causes be to forget the message the picture is trying to convey.
ReplyDeleteMarcel, thanks for sharing this link. I absolutely agree. Tobacco is a great example of the influence of transnational corporations. It seems like Big Tobacco has more influence in the United States compared to other countries (where the warning labels were implemented).
ReplyDeleteShelly, I think you bring up an interesting issue as well. I think the extremely gross images may cause many people to avoid the ads, which makes them ineffective. I am not sure people can process images that are too disturbing without getting defensive or shutting down.